In re: Elysium Health-ChromaDex Litigation, No. 17-cv-7394
(LJL), 2022 WL 421135 (S.D.N.Y. Feb. 11, 2022)
So very much going on here, including discussion of what makes controls appropriate and the challenges of surveying for materiality; I will try to skip as much
detail as possible but this is roughly 200 pages of opinions. Plaintiff
ChromaDex sought to exclude defendant Elysium’s survey expert and damages
rebuttal expert, while Elysium moved to exclude ChromaDex’s survey, damages,
FDA regulation, and clinical studies experts. Each motion was granted in part
and denied in part.
Starting with the survey experts: Excluding survey evidence
requires meeting a “heavy burden,” as long as a survey is “fairly prepared and
its results directed to relevant issues.” As a general matter, “[e]rrors in
methodology ... properly go only to the weight of the evidence” and not to its
admissibility.
Elysium’s expert, Sowers, tested the allegation that
ChromaDex advertising deceived customers into believing that the FDA reviewed
ChromaDex’s product, Tru Niagen, for efficacy. He surveyed “potential
purchasers of products to support cellular health.” The test group saw homepage
statements regarding FDA involvement in Tru Niagen, including the phrases “3
FDA Safety Notifications” and “reviewed and accepted by ... US FDA.” The
control group was shown the same stimulus but with the following disclaimer
added: “The Food and Drug Administration has not reviewed Tru Niagen for
effectiveness.”
The key question was “Does the webpage communicate anything
about whether or not the FDA has reviewed the Tru Niagen product for
effectiveness?” Those who ultimately said yes and that the FDA had reviewed it
were asked “What do you believe effectiveness means in this context?” The
expert concluded that 23.3% of respondents, net, received the message that the
FDA had reviewed Tru Niagen for effectiveness.
Elysium’s survey/expert was not excluded on Daubert
grounds, but the court rejected Elysium’s challenges to ChromaDex’s statements “that
do no more than convey the FDA status of ChromaDex’s products,” so the survey
was no longer relevant. Still, the court’s rejection of challenges to the
survey may be of interest.
ChromaDex argued that Sowers should not have proceeded to
ask respondents directly whether they believed the FDA had reviewed Tru Niagen
for effectiveness after none of the respondents identified that as the “main”
message conveyed by the webpage or thought to mention it as another message
that was conveyed by the webpage. But closed-ended questions are acceptable
even if respondents don’t mention something in response to open-ended questions;
they may “remind respondents of options that they would not otherwise consider
or which simply do not come to mind as easily.” The survey asked whether the
website conveyed anything about whether the FDA reviewed the product for
safety, and allowed both no and don’t know answers.
ChromaDex then argued that the survey asked about the FDA
and effectiveness too much. But only a respondent who said that the website did
say something about that would get later questions, “and those questions too
used the terms no more than necessary to convey the query clearly, directly,
and impartially. The option to select ‘Don’t know/unsure’ ensured that each
survey question was not overly suggestive.”
Quibbles over the study population—it used people looking to
purchase products supporting cellular health, whereas ChromaDex wanted to limit
the population to dietary supplements and expand it to people looking for other
benefits as well—went only to weight.
Turning to the competing study: ChromaDex’s expert,
Isaacson, tested two versions of Elysium’s homepage, a video stating, “Inside
this bottle is 25 years of research”; the statement “We conduct rigorous safety
studies for a new dietary ingredient (NDI) submission to the FDA”; and a social
media post that displays a jar of Basis and states, “Basis is clinically proven
to increase NAD+ levels, which decline with age.”
test/control stimuli |
His survey population differed somewhat as did the control group/questions. While the Elysium survey asked an open-ended question about whether the stimulus communicated anything about a particular subject (e.g., whether the stimulus conveyed anything about safety), Isaacson’s survey “asked a more leading question by framing a particular proposition or statement (e.g., that clinical trials demonstrated that the supplement was safe) and asking whether the communication at issue communicated or implied that statement.” Each respondent was asked about three or four statements, including one test statement and two or three distractor statements. Isaacson also asked about test cell respondents about materiality: “If you learned that the statement below is not true, would that change your likelihood of purchasing this supplement?” For materiality, the statements “The supplement described on the [material] is available in a white container” and “The supplement described on the [material] is also sold in Canada” were used as controls.
Isaacson concluded that: (1) a net percentage of 13% of the
respondents who were shown the test 2019 homepage believed that it communicated
or implied that clinical trials had demonstrated that Elysium’s product was
safe; (2) a net percentage of 38.9% of the respondents believed that the test
Facebook page and video communicated or implied that the company conducted
twenty-five years of research on aging; (3) a net percentage of 21.9% of the
respondents who were shown the test 2017 homepage and mission page believed
that it communicated or implied that Elysium submitted an NDI to the FDA; and
(4) a net percentage of 32.4% of the respondents who were shown the test post believed
that it communicated or implied that Elysium’s product was clinically proven to
slow the effects of aging. He also concluded that the statements were material.
The court concluded that there was no basis for the expert
to opine on whether there was substantial deception in the aggregate; each
challenged statement was different and its falsity/materiality needed to be
established separately, although the study’s relevant conclusions might extend
to a statement individually similar to one of the four challenged statements.
Elysium also challenged the materiality survey in its
entirety. It argued that the questions were leading because materiality
questions were asked only after the deception questions, drawing their
attention to something they might not otherwise have noticed or considered
relevant. In addition, the materiality portion used different controls, so a
respondent could recognize that the statement remaining consistent between the
two sections—the challenged statement—was the statement being tested. While
surveys should control for demand effects, the surveys here didn’t suggest to
consumers what the “right” answer was, only what the relevant topic was. “[T]he
respondents did not know what the survey sponsor wanted to hear and what the
survey sought to demonstrate and thus were not more likely to provide that
response.”
The court was given more pause by the fact that respondents
were asked about materiality only after they were asked questions about
deception that drew their attention to a particular message, causing “focalism,”
that is, causing respondents to pay “more attention to a product attribute than
they would during the purchasing process,” and increasing the value they placed
on that attribute. ChromaDex argued that this was a natural result of asking
appropriate filter questions. But it was possible that the questions could both
serve as filters and draw respondents’ attention to a particular message “such
that the message took on disproportionate significance in their minds.” And the
controls didn’t address that flaw. The controls, with their innocuous subject
matter, did catch respondents “who would reflexively answer that learning the
message was false would affect their purchasing decision about any message
about the product, no matter how innocuous.” But the controls were bad for
another reason: they showed up only in the second portion of the survey,
whereas respondents had already been asked about the test messages. That is, “respondents
were shown the control messages and immediately asked whether learning those
messages were untrue would impact their purchasing decisions, without the
intermediate steps of viewing an advertising statement, being asked whether
that statement conveys a particular message, and then being asked materiality
questions about that particular message. Respondents were never shown an
advertising statement conveying the control message.” This wasn’t a good
control because it wasn’t as similar to the experimental stimulus as possible.
It didn’t correct for focalism, and, because they hadn’t seen ads with the
controls, the controls didn’t account for respondents who would change
purchasing behavior “only because they felt deceived, rather than because the
subject matter of the statement tested was important to them.”
The court wasn’t persuaded that these flaws did affect the
survey responses, but it was persuaded that it was impossible to tell whether
that had happened.
Elysium then argued that the challenged statements being
tested contained multiple facts, but respondents were just asked generally: “If
you learned that the statement below is not true, would that change your
likelihood of purchasing this supplement?” E.g., when the challenged statement
was “The company described in the Facebook page and video conducted 25 years of
research on aging,” it wasn’t clear whether consumers thought another company
did some of the research, another company did all of the research, there was no
research, there was only 20 years of research, the research did not pertain to
aging, etc. So what was being tested for materiality? “The plaintiff has to
prove both that the message is untrue or misleading and that the particular
message that is untrue or misleading is material.” It was easy to see why it
could be important to a consumer that there wasn’t 25 years of research, but
not important that one company did that research. By contrast, Isaacson’s
survey tested “Inside this bottle is 25 years of research,” against the
relevant control “Inside this bottle is 25 years of research, conducted by us
and others.”
Ultimately, the flaws in the materiality survey were too
great for admissibility.
Damages experts: The court began with a statement that a
Lanham Act [false advertising] plaintiff must prove causation to get damages.
ChromaDex’s damages expert was excluded for assuming without
justification that, but for the alleged wrongful acts, Elysium would have made
no sales of its Basis product and the Basis product would be removed from the
market. Based on their status as direct competitors, he opined that all of
Elysium’s sales would instead have been made by ChromaDex. This case was
distinguishable from a more forgiving one (Church & Dwight, the pregnancy
duration case) in which the parties owned the top two brands and the false advertising
was of the key feature of the products. “This case by contrast involves many
advertisements with different themes, none of which ran throughout the launch
of the product; there is no evidence that they pertained to the key
distinguishing feature between the products or that, indeed, they had salience
at all in attracting consumers to Basis.”
ChromaDex’s FDA expert’s general testimony regarding
relevant FDA regulatory schemes was admissible because it “may help a jury
understand unfamiliar terms and concepts,” though a few scattered parts were
excluded for being conclusory/otherwise not within proper scope.
No comments:
Post a Comment